Литмир - Электронная Библиотека

Gergely, György, and Gergely Csibra. ‘Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naïve theory of rational action’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7.7 (2003): 287—92

Gergely, György, Harold Bekkering and Ildikó Király. ‘Developmental psychology: rational imitation in preverbal infants’, Nature 415.6873 (2002): 755

Hamlin, J. Kiley, and Karen Wynn. ‘Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others’, Cognitive Development 26.1 (2011): 30—39

Hamlin, J. Kiley, Karen Wynn and Paul Bloom. ‘Social evaluation by preverbal infants’, Nature 450.7169 (2007): 557—9

Hamlin, J. Kiley, et al. ‘Not like me = bad: infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others’, Psychological Science (2013): 589—94

Hood, Bruce, Susan Carey and Sandeep Prasada. ‘Predicting the outcomes of physical events: two-year-olds fail to reveal knowledge of solidity and support’, Child Development 71.6 (2000): 1540—54

Izard, Véronique, et al. ‘Newborn infants perceive abstract numbers’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 106.25 (2009): 10382—5

Kinzler, Katherine D., Emmanuel Dupoux and Elizabeth S. Spelke. ‘The native language of social cognition’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 104.30 (2007): 12577—80

Kuhl, Patricia K. ‘Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 5.11 (2004): 831—43

Meltzoff, Andrew N., and M. Keith Moore. ‘Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates’, Science 198.4312 (1977): 75—8

Meltzoff, Andrew N., and Richard W. Borton. ‘Intermodal matching by human neonates’, Nature 282.5737 (1979): 403—4

Mesz, Bruno, Marcos A. Trevisan and Mar ano Sigman. ‘The taste of music’, Perception 40.2 (2011): 209—19

Núñez, Rafael E., and Eve Sweetser. ‘With the future behind them: convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time’, Cognitive Science 30.3 (2006): 401—50

Posner, Michael I., and Stanislas Dehaene. ‘Attentional networks’, Trends in Neurosciences 17.2 (1994): 75—9

Ramachandran, Vilayanur S., and Edward M. Hubbard. ‘Synaesthesia — a window into perception, thought and language’, Journal of Consciousness Studies 8.12 (2001): 3—34

Ramus, Franck, et al. ‘Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys’, Science 288.5464 (2000): 349—51

Rochat, Philippe. Early Social Cognition: Understanding Others in the First Months of Life. Psychology Press, 2014

Rueda, M. Rosario, et al. ‘Development of attentional networks in childhood’, Neuropsychologia 42.8 (2004): 1029—40

Saffran, Jenny R., Richard N. Aslin and Elissa L. Newport. ‘Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants’, Science 274.5294 (1996) 1926—8

Schulz, Laura, Tamar Kushnir and Alison Gopnik. ‘Learning from doing: intervention and causal inference’, Causal Learning: Psychology, Philosophy, and Computation (2007): 67—85

Sigman, Mariano, et al. ‘Neuroscience and education: prime time to build the bridge’, Nature Neuroscience 17.4 (2014): 497—502

Valvo, Alberto. Sight Restoration after Long Term Blindness: The Problems and Behavior Patterns of Visual Rehabilitation. American Foundation for the Blind, 1971

Розділ 2. Туманні межі ідентичності

Ais, Joaquín, et al. ‘Individual consistency in the accuracy and distribution of confidence judgments’, Cognition 146 (2016): 377—86

Ariely, Dan, and George Loewenstein. ‘The heat of the moment: the effect of sexual arousal on sexual decision making’, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making 19.2 (2006): 87—98

Barttfeld, Pablo, et al. ‘Distinct patterns of functional brain connectivity correlate with objective performance and subjective beliefs’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110.28 (2013): 11577—82

Bechara, Antoine, et al. ‘Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy’, Science 275.5304 (1997): 1293—5

Brodsky, Warren. Driving With Music: Cognitive-Behavioural Implications. Ashgate Publishing, 2015

Cesarini, David, et al. ‘Heritability of cooperative behavior in the trust game’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105.10 (2008): 3721—6

Chaim, and Uri Gneez. ‘Discrimination in a segmented society: an experimental approach’, The Quarterly Journal of Economics 116.1 (2001): 351—77

Charness, Gary, and Uri Gneezy. ‘What’s in a name? Anonymity and social distance in dictator and ultimatum games’, Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization 68.1 (2008): 29—35

Costa, Albert, et al. ‘Your morals depend on language’, PloS One 9.4 (2014): e94842

Croson, Rachel, and Nancy Buchan. ‘Gender and culture: international experimental evidence from trust games’, The American Economic Review 89.2 (1999): 386—91

Croson, Rachel, and Uri Gneezy. ‘Gender differences in preferences’, Journal of Economic Literature 47.2 (2009): 448—74

Danziger, Shai, Jonathan Levav and Liora Avnaim-Pesso. ‘Extraneous factors in judicial decisions’, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108.17 (2011): 6889—92

Dehaene, Stanislas, et al. ‘Imaging unconscious semantic priming’, Nature 395.6702 (1998): 597—600

Delgado, Maur cio R., Robert H. Frank and Elisabeth A. Phelps. ‘Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game’, Nature Neuroscience 8.11 (2005): 1611—18

Di Tella, Rafael, et al. ‘Conveniently upset: avoiding altruism by distorting beliefs about others’ altruism’, The American Economic Review 105.11 (2015): 3416—42

Dijksterhuis, Ap, et al. ‘On making the right choice: the deliberation-without-attention effect’, Science 311.5763 (2006): 1005—7

Drugowitsch, Jan, et al. ‘Tuning the speed-accuracy trade-off to maximize reward rate in multisensory decision-making’, Elife 4 (2015): e06678

Fleming, Stephen M., et al. ‘Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure’, Science 329.5998 (2010): 1541—3

Gold, Joshua I., and Michael N. Shadlen. ‘Banburismus and the brain: decoding the relationship between sensory stimuli, decisions, and reward’, Neuron 36.2 (2002): 299—308

Graziano, Martin, and Mariano Sigman. ‘The spatial and temporal construction of confidence in the visual scene’, PLoS One 4.3 (2009): e4909

Greene, Joshua, and Jonathan Haidt. ‘How (and where) does moral judgment work?’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6.12 (2002): 517—23

Greene, Joshua D., et al. ‘An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment’, Science 293.5537 (2001): 2105—8

Guastella, Adam J., and Ian B. Hickie. ‘Oxytocin treatment, circuitry, and autism: a critical review of the literature placing oxytocin into the autism context’, Biological Psychiatry 79.3 (2016): 234—42

Haley, Kevin J., and Daniel M. T. Fessler. ‘Nobody’s watching?: Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game’, Evolution and Human Behavior 26.3 (2005): 245—56

Hall, Joshua C. Homer Economicus: The Simpsons and Economics. Stanford University Press, 2014

Hanks, Timothy D., Jochen Ditterich and Michael N. Shadlen. ‘Microstimulation of macaque area LIP affects decision-making in a motion discrimination task’, Nature Neuroscience 9.5 (2006): 682—9

Henrich, Joseph, et al. ‘In search of homo economicus: behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies’, The American Economic Review 91.2 (2001): 73—8

Johnson, Eric J., and Daniel Goldstein. ‘Medicine. Do defaults save lives?’, Science 302.5649 (2003): 1338—9

Kahneman, Daniel. Thinking, Fast and Slow. Macmillan, 2011

Kosfeld, Michael, et al. ‘Oxytocin increases trust in humans’, Nature 435.7042 (2005): 673—6

Leone, María J., et al. ‘The tell-tale heart: heart rate fluctuations index objective and subjective events during a game of chess’, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience (2012): doi 10.3389/fnhum. 2012.00273

Miller, Greg. ‘The promise and perils of oxytocin’, Science 339.6117 (2013): 267—9

51
{"b":"831830","o":1}